Welcome to The Visible Embryo
  o
 
The Visible Embryo Home
   
Google  
Home--- -History-----Bibliography-----Pregnancy Timeline-----Prescription Drugs in Pregnancy---- Pregnancy Calculator----Female Reproductive System----News----Contact
 
Welcome to The Visible Embryo, a comprehensive educational resource on human development from conception to birth.

The Visible Embryo provides visual references for changes in fetal development throughout pregnancy and can be navigated via fetal development or maternal changes.

The National Institutes of Child Health and Human Development awarded Phase I and Phase II Small Business Innovative Research Grants to develop The Visible Embryo. Initally designed to evaluate the internet as a teaching tool for first year medical students, The Visible Embryo is linked to over 600 educational institutions and is viewed by more than one million visitors each month.

Today, The Visible Embryo is linked to over 600 educational institutions and is viewed by more than 1 million visitors each month. The field of early embryology has grown to include the identification of the stem cell as not only critical to organogenesis in the embryo, but equally critical to organ function and repair in the adult human. The identification and understanding of genetic malfunction, inflammatory responses, and the progression in chronic disease, begins with a grounding in primary cellular and systemic functions manifested in the study of the early embryo.

WHO International Clinical Trials Registry Platform


The World Health Organization (WHO) has created a new Web site to help researchers, doctors and patients obtain reliable information on high-quality clinical trials. Now you can go to one website and search all registers to identify clinical trial research underway around the world!





Home

History

Bibliography

Pregnancy Timeline

Prescription Drug Effects on Pregnancy

Pregnancy Calculator

Female Reproductive System

News

Disclaimer: The Visible Embryo web site is provided for your general information only. The information contained on this site should not be treated as a substitute for medical, legal or other professional advice. Neither is The Visible Embryo responsible or liable for the contents of any websites of third parties which are listed on this site.


Content protected under a Creative Commons License.
No dirivative works may be made or used for commercial purposes.

 

Pregnancy Timeline by SemestersDevelopmental TimelineFertilizationFirst TrimesterSecond TrimesterThird TrimesterFirst Thin Layer of Skin AppearsEnd of Embryonic PeriodEnd of Embryonic PeriodFemale Reproductive SystemBeginning Cerebral HemispheresA Four Chambered HeartFirst Detectable Brain WavesThe Appearance of SomitesBasic Brain Structure in PlaceHeartbeat can be detectedHeartbeat can be detectedFinger and toe prints appearFinger and toe prints appearFetal sexual organs visibleBrown fat surrounds lymphatic systemBone marrow starts making blood cellsBone marrow starts making blood cellsInner Ear Bones HardenSensory brain waves begin to activateSensory brain waves begin to activateFetal liver is producing blood cellsBrain convolutions beginBrain convolutions beginImmune system beginningWhite fat begins to be madeHead may position into pelvisWhite fat begins to be madePeriod of rapid brain growthFull TermHead may position into pelvisImmune system beginningLungs begin to produce surfactant
CLICK ON weeks 0 - 40 and follow along every 2 weeks of fetal development




 

Multilingual babies use more cognitive control

You may believe that you have forgotten the Chinese you spoke as a child, but your brain hasn't. Moreover, that "forgotten" first language may well influence what goes on in your brain when you speak English or French today. New research demonstrates our brain's plasticity and ability to adapt to new language environments.


In a paper published today in Nature Communications, researchers from McGill University and the Montreal Neurological Institute describe their discovery that even brief, early exposure to a language influences how the brain processes sounds from a second language later in life. Even when the first language learned is no longer spoken.

It is an important finding because this research tells scientists both about how the brain becomes wired for language, but also about how that hardwiring can change and adapt over time in response to new language environments.


The research has implications for our understanding of how brain plasticity functions, and may also be important information about creating educational practices geared to different types of learners.


Researchers asked three groups of children (aged 10 - 17) with very different linguistic backgrounds to identify French pseudo-words (such as vapagne and chansette).

One group of children was born and raised in unilingual French-speaking families. The second group were adopted from China into a French-speaking family before age three, stopped speaking Chinese, and from that point on heard and used only French. The third group were fluently bilingual in Chinese and French.

As the children responded to the words they heard, the researchers used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to look at which parts of their brains were being activated.

Although all groups performed the tasks equally well, the areas of the brain that were activated differed between groups.


In monolingual French children without exposure to Chinese, the left inferior frontal gyrus and anterior insula areas of the brain, expected to be involved in processing language-associated sounds were activated.

However, children who are bilingual (Chinese/French) and those who had been exposed to Chinese as young infants but stopped speaking it, use additional areas: the right middle frontal gyrus, left medial frontal cortexand bilateral superior temporal gyrus are all active.

It appears early language sounds leave brain traces.


Researchers found that the Chinese children who had been adopted into unilingual French families and no longer spoke Chinese, were functionally unilingual at the time of testing, but still had brains that processed language in a way similar to bilingual children.

"During the first year of life, a first step in language development in an infants' brain is to collect and store "important sounds" relative to what is said around them," said Lara Pierce, a doctoral student at McGill and the first author on the article.

"What we discovered from children adopted into French-language families - who no longer spoke Chinese, they like bilingual children, showed brain activation in areas known to be involved in working memory and cognitive control during language tests.

"These results suggest children exposed to Chinese as infants process French in a different manner to monolingual French children."


Researchers believe their findings speak to the unique and lasting influence of early language experience on later brain organization.


Pierce: "Adopted children we tested have an interesting background as they were exposed to one language from birth, but completely discontinued that language when adopted into families who speak a new language.

"This is very interesting from a language development perspective because it allows us to compare just that very early language development on later language processing."

Future research will be directed at whether similar areas of the brain are active if languages that had been "lost" and "gained" through adoption are more similar to each other — such as with French and Spanish.

Abstract
Early experiences may establish a foundation for later learning, however, influences of early language experience on later neural processing are unknown. We investigated whether maintenance of neural templates from early language experience influences subsequent language processing. Using fMRI, we scanned the following three groups performing a French phonological working memory (PWM) task: (1) monolingual French children; (2) children adopted from China before age 3 who discontinued Chinese and spoke only French; (3) Chinese-speaking children who learned French as a second language but maintained Chinese. Although all groups perform this task equally well, brain activation differs. French monolinguals activate typical PWM brain regions, while both Chinese-exposed groups also activate regions implicated in cognitive control, even the adoptees who were monolingual French speakers at testing. Early exposure to a language, and/or delayed exposure to a subsequent language, continues to influence the neural processing of subsequently learned language sounds years later even in highly proficient, early-exposed users.

Return to top of page

Dec 29, 2015   Fetal Timeline   Maternal Timeline   News   News Archive   



Imaging slices are shown overlapping in the axial plane to demonstrate
the similarity between brains of bilingual and internationally adopted children.
The left hemisphere is on the left side, bilinguals (RED), international adoptees
(BLUE), and monolinguals (GREEN). Bilingual children, showed brain activation
in areas known to be involved in working memory and cognitive control
.

Image Credit: Lara J. Pierce, Jen-Kai Chen, Audrey Delcenserie,
Fred Genesee & Denise Klein




 





 


 

Phospholid by Wikipedia